The
typology of tonal systems in the Kinande complex verb
Ngessimo M. Mutaka
Rutgers University and
University of Yaounde 1.
0. Introduction
The status of auxiliary
verbs in Bantu languages with a highly concatenative morphology has remained up
till now a conundrum for linguistic theory. It is not always clear whether the
auxiliary verb and the main verb form two maximal projections or a single
maximal projection in a syntactic tree structure. Despite the fact that the
structure of the simplex verb is relatively well understood, the unit called
INFL (i.e. subject marker and tense-aspectual marker(s)) still poses problems,
especially when the auxiliary is considered
part of this INFL. This paper seeks to provide a typology of tonal
systems used in the auxiliary and in the INFL of the complex verb in
Kinande, a language of eastern DRC
(former Zaire) in order to provide a phonological argument that certain kinds of auxiliary should be considered as
part of a higher syntactic unit that encodes the tense. It is shown that tonal
processes that are assigned as suffixal tones in the simplex (i.e. non complex)
verb typically surface in the auxiliary. At the same time, it is also shown
that the auxiliary forms and the main verb constitute a single intonational
phrase. The paper is organized as follows: §1 deals with the tonal
processes in the tense-aspectual markers (TAM) of the simplex verb, §2 deals with the tones in
the complex verb, and §3 is a summary of
the processes in both the simplex and complex verbs followed by some concluding
remarks abou the status of the auxiliary and the main verb.
However, before dealing with
the tonal processes in the simplex verb, the structure of the verb, simplex and
complex, is in order here. It is given in (1, 2).
Kinande is a typical Narrow
Bantu language whose structure of the simplex verb typically consists of the
following concatenative morphemes: SM-TAM-OM-Rt-Ext.- FV.
(The abbreviations are as
follows: SM = subject marker, TAM = tense-aspect marker, OM = object marker, Rt
= root, Ext = extension, FV = final vowel).
This structure is
illustrated in the following Kinande form.
(1) tu-náàmundisyaàtaàsyaya-ba-king-ul-ir-a
SM
TAM OM Rt Ext(s)FV
we them open
‘we
will one more time open for them’
The
verb may appear in a more complex form, that is, it may consist of the
auxiliary followed by the main verb as illusrated in (2).
(2) The structure of the complex verb:
auxiliary+ main verb.
tuà
- lyaà - byaà tuà - ka - gánda ‘we
were going’
SM
TM be SM TM stem
we be we go
1.
The tense-aspectual markers in the Kinande simplex verb
Because
Kinande has numerous TAM morphemes, let me first give their positions in the
INFL unit of the verb.
(3) Positions of the TM morphemes (Valinande
1984, Mutaka 1994)
1
2 3 4
5 6 7 8 9
10 11 12 13
a
nga na ma bi ri
e ne mu ndi ta
sya ya
ka
ki
ø
Some
morphemes have fixed H tones, others are toneless and others have a regular H
tone that anticipates onto the preceding vowel and delinks from its sponsor.
The typology of these TAMs is presented in (4).
(4) Types of tones in the TMs (Mutaka
1994)
Fixed tones Toneless Regular
tones
ka na li (lya)
ndi si (sya)
si
(sya) ta
ya amaa
ne (nendi) ne (nendi) bi
abïùri li
(limu)
mu
(limu)
1.1. The simplex verb with one TAM in the
INFL unit.
Let
us first examine the tense on single tense markers that appear in the simplex
verb. The verb roots in (a,b) is toneless whereas the ones in (c,d) is H. An
example with an OM is also given to show the changes that occurs when an OM is
part of the expanded stem.
(5)a. tw-a-humiraè /tu-a-hum-ir-aC-aL/ we hit for (in the remote past)
tw-a-mu-humira we hit for
him (remote)
tw-a-lusaàyaè we took
off our clothes
b. tw-aà-huàmiraè
/tu-a-hum-ir-aC-aHL/ we hit for
(in the recent past)
tw-a-muà-huàmiraè ‘we hit for him
(recent past)
tw-aà-luàsaàyaè
we
took off our clothes
c. tw-aà-tum-ïraè
/tu-a-tum-ir-aC-aL/ we sent for (remote past)
tw-a-mu-tumiraè we sent for
him (remote past)
d. tw-a-tum-ïùraà-aè
/tu-a-tum-ir-aC-aHL/ we sent for (recent past)
tw-aà-muà-tum-ïùraà-aè we sent for
him (recent past)
The
TM -a- is underlyingly toneless. For a toneless verb root, no tone surfaces in
the entire form as illustrated in (a) whereas a H surfaces on the root and
pre-root vowel in the recent past tense as illustrated in (b). For a H tone
verb root, a H surfaces on the TM -a- in a remote past tense just in case there
is no OM in the form, otherwise, it surfaces with a L if there is an OM in the
form. As for the recent past tense, the -a- surfaces with no tone if there is
no OM in the stem, otherwise it surfaces with a H tone that anticipates from
the OM. Notice that the suffixal HL assigned by the recent past tense surface
in the stem as illustrated in (c,d). Consider also the following forms:
(6) tu-kaà-gándaê /tu-ka-gánd-aC-a/ we are about to go
tu-kaà-lusayaê we
are taking off our clothes
The
H on kaà is fixed. It neither anticipates to the preceding vowel nor delinks
from its sponsor.
(7)
a. tuà-ki-gándaê /tu-ka-gánd-aC-a/ while we are still going
tuà-ki-lusayaê while we
are still taking off our clothes
b. tuà-li-gándaê… if we had gone …
nyïù-ri-gándaà
… if I had gone …
nyïù-ri-lusayaà
… if I had taken of my clothes …
c. tuà-sya-gándaê
let us go one day
tuà-sya-lusayaê let us go and take off
our clothes
An
examination of the forms in (6) indicates that the SM is toneless. The H on the
SM in these forms in (7) must therefore originate from the TMs -ki-, -li-,
sya. The l/r alternation in (7b) is due to the quality of the round vowel that
precedes. For an account, see Mutaka (1987). Consider the following forms:
(8)
a. tu-taà-gándaâaè let us go Informational
imperative[1]
tu-taà-lusyaâaè let us take off our
clothes please
tu-taà-humiraâaè let us hit please
b. tu-taà-tumaâaè let us send
c. erï-humïù:ra to hit for
erïù-ta-humïù:ra to merely hit for
erinaàtahumïùr:ra to just merely hit for
The
H that surfaces on -ta- is presumably fixed like the one in -ka- although,
underlyingly, it is associated to a H that anticipates onto the preceding vowel
and delinks from its sponsor as illustrated in the forms in (c ). Notice that
this
-ta- is part of a discontinuous morpheme ta … a. At this stage, it is not clear
how the contour tone gets onto the prefinal vowel. Consider also the following
forms:
(9)
a. ta-gándaâaè go please
ta-lusyaâaè take off your clothes
ta-humiraâaè hit for please
b. taà-tumaâaè send please
The
-ta- in this subjunctive form must be toneless as only the lexical H on the H
tone verb surfaces on it as shown in (b). This -ta- is also part of the
discontinuous morpheme ta…a as in the previous example.
(10)a. tuà-ya-gáànd-árár-á let us go in the very near future on purpose.
tuà-yaà-luàsiriraàyá let us go and take off our clothes on
purpose
tuà-yaà-huàm-ir-á let us go and hit
b. tuà-ya-tuàmáê let us go and send
tuà-ya-tum-ïùráê let us go and send for
c. eri-ya-hum-ïù:ra to go and hit.
The
TM -ya- in these forms derives from the verb ári-ya-a ‘to go’. It presumably
gets a suffixal H that surfaces on the vowel that precedes it as shown in the H
tone verb root in (b). The subjunctive form assigns a suffixal HL in these
forms. The suffixal H surfaces on the root and pre-root vowel of a toneless verb
root as in (a) and on the penultimate and antepenultimate vowels of a H tone
verb root as in (b). The lack of a H on -ya- in (10a) still needs an
explanation that we do not have at this stage of our work. It is due to the
irregularity of the verb ári-gánda ‘to go’. The fact that a H tone appears on
the SM of forms in (a) indicates that the suffixal H both targets the root
vowel and the TAM -ya- from which it anticipates. The TAM -ya- is toneless
underlyingly as illustrated in (c ).
To
summarize so far, the H on a single TM may be fixed as on -ka-, it may
anticipate onto a preceding vowel and delink from its sponsor as on -ki-, and
it may be a suffixal H that targets the TM as on -ya-. Concerning the
imperative forms using the TAM -ya-, a suffixal H of the HL melody assigned by
the tense targets both -ya- and the expected TBUs in the verb stem, that is,
the first root vowel in a toneless verb root and the penultimate vowel in a H
tone verb root. This summary is made
clearer in the following tableau with illustrative examples.
TAM
in INFL |
Stem |
fixed H on -ka- tu-kaà- |
no special suffixal tone humiraà-a |
H
anticipates & delinks from sponsor tuà-kina |
no special suffixal tone humiraà-a |
suffixal HL targeting -ya- tuà-yaà- |
suffixal HL huàmiráè |
special status of -a- in a H tone
verb: a in recent past w/t OM: e.g. tu-a- aà
with OM: e.g. tu-aà- |
suffixal HL tum-ïùraà-aè muà-tumïùraà-aè |
special status of -a- in a H tone
verb: aà in remote past w/t OM: e.g.
tu-aà- a with
OM e.g. tu-a |
suffixal L tumiraè mu-tumiraè |
regular H of -ta- becomes fixed in
informational imperative: e.g. tu-taà- |
a LHL melody on the FV humiraâaè |
1.2. The simplex verb with two TAMs in the
INFL unit.
Let
us now examine the tone on the TAM that consists of two morphemes, bearing in
mind that, from a syntactic point of view, it is more likely that it is the TAM
that is first in the sequence that is the tense marker.
(11)a. tu-ama-gáànda if
we leave
tu-ama-hum-irïùr-a if we hit on purpose
b. tu-aàmaê-gáànda we are leaving
tu-aàmaà-gánd-áráàr-a we are leaving on purpose
Whereas
the -ama- in (a) is toneless, the -ama- in (b) is presumably -amaa- and is associated with a branching H. It is not
clear whether this branching H is fixed on -ama- or is on -aa- of amaa and that
it undergoes H tone anticipation followed by delinking of the rightmost branch.
Consider the following form:
(12) tu-asya-gánda we came to leave (in the remote past)
In
this form, a L tone of the remote past tense should presumably be posited on
the final vowel of the verb. The two TAMs are therefore toneless. Consider also
the following forms:
(13)a. tu-aàya-gándaê we
went there to leave (Recent past)
tu-aàya-humiriraê we went to hit on purpose
b. tu-aàyaà-tum-ir-aê we went there to send
The
H on -a- of -aya- is presumably the suffixal H of the recent past tense which
is assigned onto -ya- which, as pointed out previously, is a verb that acts as
a tense marker. Curiously enough, this suffixal H does not target the vowels in
the stem. Notice that the falling tone on the FV is the result of a phrasal H
and the intonational L. Consider also the following forms:
(14)a.
tu-andi-humïù:ra we would hit for
tu-andi-humira Maguà:lu we would hit for Magulu
b. tu-aàndi-humïù:ra we would have hit for
tu-aàndi-humira Maguà:lu we would have hit for Magulu
Whereas
no underlying tone should be posited in the TAMs in (a), it is not clear
whether the H on aà of -andi- is the result of a H tone anticipation followed
by delinking from the sponsor or if it is assigned onto -a- in these forms.
What is certain is that forms in (b) express a recent past wereas those in (a)
a remote past tense. The H in (b) can thus be equated to the H found in the
recent past tense. In this case, it seems safer to say that it directly targets
the vowel -a-. One possibility is that this tense marker -a- is stressed in
these forms and thus attracts tone. Consider also the following forms:
(15)a.
tu-lya-gándaê /tu-li-a-gánd-aC-a/ if
we were to leave
b. tuà-lya-gándaê /tu-li-a-gánd-aC-a/ we left
Assuming
that -lya- derives from -li-a-, and that this -li- is the same as the one for
which we posited an underlying H that anticipates and delink, we must assume
that the lack of a H tone in the form expressing irrealis in (a) is the result
of a rule or a constraint banning the surfacing of a H in such a form.
(16). tu-kaàndi-gáànd-a we will go
tu-kaàna-gándaê
we sometimes go
tu-kaàbi-gáànda we left ( a short time ago)
tu-kaàta-gándaê we are leaving (for now)
tu-kaàsya-gándaê we will leave (one day)
tu-kaàya-gándaê
we are going to leave
These
forms illustrate the fixed H on -ka-; in other words, the H on -ka- does not
anticipate and delink from its sponsor. Consider also the following forms:
(17)a. tu-kinaà-huàmiraè we are still hitting
for
b. tuà-kina-humiraê while we are still hitting for
While
the H on -naà- in (a) is the result of the suffixal H in this tense, the H on
the SM in (b) presumably derives from -ki- which is part of a discontinuous
morpheme ki-na- …a. However, since the SM of the form in (a) does not surface
with a H that would anticipate from an underlying H on -ki-, it is possible
that the H on the SM in (b) is morphologically assigned in this form.
(18). tu-ándi-gáànda we would have left (in a remote past)
tu-áàndi-gáànda we would have left (in a more recent
past)
These
forms are similar to those in (14). As they contrast a recent and remote past,
it is plausible that the suffixal L tone of the remote past is responsible for
the L tone in (a) and that the suffixal H of the recent past tense is also
responsible for the H tone in (b). Consider the following form:
(19). tu-náàmu-gáànda we are leaving
The
H on náà is presumably fixed, if we assume that it is the same morpheme that
appears in a form like tu-náà buyira ‘we have nothing’ (literally: we are
nothing).
To
summarize, the tones that appear in a TAM that consists of two morphemes
include the following tonological processes:
(a) a H tone that remains fixed on a specific
morpheme; e.g. náà of -náàmu-; kaà in kaàsya, kaàna, etc.
(b)
a H tone that anticipates onto the preceding TBU and delinks from its sponsor;
e.g. the H on the SM tuà- in tuà-kina-gándaê.
(c
) a suffixal H tone that is typically assigned by the recent past tense and
which surfaces on the TBU that sponsors that particular tense marker; e.g. in
tu-aàya-gándaê ‘we recently went to leave’
(d)
a L tone that is typically assigned by a remote past tense and whose effect is
a surface L tone onto the TAM; e.g. tu-ándi-gáànda ‘we would have left’
Since
the typology of tones in the forms with two TAMs is not very different from the
one we encountered in the forms with a single TAM, I will only list the
processes that do not figure in the previous tableau.
Tone in INFL with 2 TAMs |
Stem |
suffixal H on -ya- tu-aà-ya- |
no special tone humiriraê |
Branching H in -ama- e.g. tu-aàmaà- |
no special tone humirïù:ra |
Loss of branching H in -ama- in an if-clause e.g. tu-ama- |
no special tone humirïù:ra |
In
the following section, we not only describe the forms used as auxiliary but
also point out the tones used in the auxiliary and the TAM in the verb.
2. The tones in the complex verb form.
As
pointed out earlier, complex verb form means a form that consists
of an auxiliary and the main verb as illustrated in the following examples.
(20)a. tuà-lwáà tuà-kaà-lyaê ‘we were eating’
tu-naà-lwáà tuà-kaà-lyaê ‘we were indeed eating’
b. tuà-lwáà
tuà-ka-huàma ‘we
were hitting’
As
illustrated in these forms, the complex verb consists of an auxiliary whose
structure is SM-lwáà and the main verb whose structure is SM-TAM-Stem. Since
the auxiliary verb derives from the verb ári-lu-a, which is a toneless verb
root, we must assume that the H on the SM is the result of a suffixal H usually
found in the recent past tense (cf. Mutaka 1994). It will be observed that the
H on lwáà results from a postlexical H spreading from the initial TBU of the
following word.
What
is new in the main verb is that the fixed H on the TAM -ka- spreads onto the
preceding vowel and delinks from its sponsor. This H thus behaves like a the H
in a relative clause affirmative (Hyman, p.c.) in a form such as
oyuà-ka-humaà-a ‘the one who is hitting’. Consider also the following forms:
(21)a. tuà-lwáà ïù-tu-naà-muà-lyaê ‘we were eating’
tu-naà-lwáà ïù-tu-naà-muà-lyaê ‘we were indeed eating’
b. tu-naà-lwáà
ïù-tu-naà-mu-huàma ‘we were indeed hitting’
These
forms illustrate a second type of the use of the auxiliary -lwáà where the main
verb starts with a complementizer ïù-. Presumably, the H on -naà- is fixed in
the TAM of the main verb as -mu- has been observed to be underlyingly toneless
in simplex verbs. Actually, this naà is the same as the morpheme -náà- used also
in combination with -mu-, (e.g. tuà-lwáà ïù-tu-náàmu-huàma ‘we were hitting’).
Consider also the following forms:
(22)a. tuàlwáà ïù-tu-abïùrïù-ryaê ‘we had already eaten’
nyïù-lwáà ïù-n-abïùrïù-ryaê ‘I had already eaten’
aà-lwáà ïù-n-abïùrïù-ryaê ‘he had already eaten.
b. tuàlwáà ïù-tu-abïùrï-huàma ‘we had already hit’
Presumably,
the H on -bïù- in the main verb of this form is fixed.
(23)a. tuàlwáà ïù-tu-naà-hakaàb-ïõùrá ‘we were in the stated of being
smeared’
b. tuàlwáà
ïù-tu-naà-huàm-iõráè ‘we had already hit’
Because
the main verb uses the suffixal aspectual marker -iõrá, the H on the root vowel
and the vowel that precedes it must be the result of a suffixal H assigned by
the recent past tense. Consider also the following forms:
(24)a. tuàlwáà ïù-móà-tu-a-n-aàmaà-hakaà:ba ‘we had just semeared ourselves’
b. tuàlwáà
ïù-móà-tu-a-n-aàmaà-humïùra ‘we
had just hit for’
tuàlwáà
ïù-móà-tu-a-n-aàmaê-huàma ‘we had just
hit’
tuàlwáà
ïù-móà-tu-a-n-aàmaà-humira Maguà:lu ‘we had just hit for Magulu’
The
main verb in these forms uses a discontinuous TAM móà..a-na-amaa-. The H on
-aàmaà- is presumably the branching H that was seen in the simplex verb onto
this morpheme. Consider also the following forms:
(25) nyïùà-lwáà w’árïù-ryaê ‘we ought to eat’
baàà-lwáà
b’árïù-ryaê ‘we ought to eat’
tuà-lwáà b’árïù-ryaê ‘We ought to eat’
These
forms show that the auxiliary -lwáà- may be followed by a main verb in the
infinitive as shown by these examples. The consonant “w-/b-” in these forms is
an agreement marker that refers to mundu “person” in the case of “w” and bandu
‘people’ in case of “b”.
To
summarize, the auxiliary lwá takes a
suffixal H that surfaces on the preceding vowel and the main verb contains
tones that may be fixed as in the case of -aàmaà-, a H that anticipates onto a
preceding vowel and delinks from its sponsor as in the morpheme -kaà-, -bïù-,
and -naà-. In case the main verb has a complementizer -ïù-, this complementizer
always surfaces with a H. One peculiarity about -ka- is that its fixed H is now
regular in that it anticipates and delinks from its sponsor.
A
brief comparison of the subjunctive form of the auxiliary lwáà (from the
toneless verb root -lu- in eri-lu-a) with the subjunctive form of eri-hum-a
which has a toneless verb root will help clarify the assignment of the suffixal
H into these forms.
(26)a. tuà-luãà-á [tuàlwá] b. tuà-huàm-á
let
us leave let
us hit
nyi-naà-luãà-á
[nyinaàlwá] nyi-naà-huàm-á
let
me me come indeed let
me hit indeed
(cf. eri-luã-a ‘to leave’) (cf.
eri-hum-a ‘to hit’)
As
shown in these forms, the suffixal H surfaces onto the root vowel and the vowel
that precedes it. Notice also that the auxiliary -lwá- may be used with an
-iõrá suffixal aspectual marker as in the following example. Consider these
forms:
(27).
moà-tu-aà-lwiõr ïù-tu-aà-huàmiraè /móà-tu-a-luã-iõrá/
‘we hit immediately’
*moà-tu-aà-lwiõrá tu-aà-huàmiraè
When
used in this tense, the form has to be followed by the complementizer ïù-.
This
auxiliary -lwáà- can also be conjugated in other forms where it is preceded by
aspectual markers as illustrated in the following forms.
(28)a. tu-kaàndi-lw’ ïù-tu-aà-huàmi:raè ‘we will hit
immediately’
b. tuà-lwe-ndi-lw’
ïù-tu- aà-huàmi:raè ‘we would hit immediately’
Finally,
we would like to mention that -lwá- can be used as an aspectual marker in a non
complex verb as illustrated below.
(29). tu-naà-lwá-ndi-humïù:ra[2]
‘we could hit for’
sïù-tuà-lwá-ndi-humïù:ra ‘we would not hit for’
So
far, we have seen the use of the auxiliary -lwáà-. Another auxiliary used in
the complex verb is -bya- as illustrated in the following forms.
(30).a. tu-anataà-byaà tuà-ka-humïù:ra ‘we
were at least hitting for’
b. tu-náàmundïù-byaà tuà-ka-huàma ‘we
will be hitting’
c. tu-náàmundi-humïù:ra ‘we will hit for’
tu-náàmundïùta-humïù:ra ‘we will at least hit for’
As
shown in these examples, both the auxiliary
-bya- “be” and the main verb
-humira ‘hit for’ take tense and aspectual markers. Because -bya- is a H tone
root, the H that surfaces on the vowel which precedes it is the lexical H. As
illustrated in (c ), -ta- is underlyingly associated to a H tone that
anticipates and delinks from its sponsor. Like in the case of lwáà, the H on
byaà results from a postlexical H
spreading from the preceding word as will be demonstrated later.
(31) tu-a-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra ‘we were
hitting for’ (in a recent past)
tu-anaà-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra ‘we were indeed
hitting for’
tu-anataà-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra ‘we were at least
hitting for’ (Remote)
tu-endïù-byaà
ïù-tu- naàmu-humïù:ra ‘we would be
hitting for’
tu-enáàndïù-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra ‘we would be hitting for’
tu-náàmundïù-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra ‘we will be
hitting for’
As
shown in these forms, the auxiliary -bya- may be followed by a main verb
introduced by the complementizer ïù-
Notice that when the main verb is not introduced by the complementizer
ïù- , the number of tense-aspectual markers it can have is restricted by the
kinds of tense-aspectual markers used in the auxiliary. Consider the following
examples:
(32)a.
tu-kaàndisyaà-byaà tuà-kaà-humïù:ra
‘we will be hitting for at that time’
tw-a-
humïù:ra ‘when we will hit for at that time’
tw-ama-humïù:ra
‘if we hit for at that time’
*tu-neàmu-
humïù:ra
*tu-abïùrï-
humïù:ra
*móà-tu-anaàmaà-
humïù:ra
b. tu-kaàndisyaà-byaà ïù-tu-neàmu-humïù:ra
‘we
will be in the act of hitting for’
ïù-tu-abïùrï-
humïù:ra
‘we
will have finished hitting for’
ïù-móà-tu-anaàmaà-humïù:ra
‘we’ll just have finished hitting for’
The
following paradigms show that the tense is mostly expressed by the auxiliary
verb.
(33)a.
tu-a-naà-byaà ïù-tu-naàmuà-lyaê (more recent)
b. tu-a-na-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmuà-lyaê (more
remote)
‘we were eating indeed’
(34)a. tu-a-byaà ïù-tu-naàmuà-lyaê (progressive, more recent)
b. tu-aà-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmuà-lyaê (more past)
‘we were eating’
(35)a. tu-aà-byaà tuà-kaà-lyaê more past
b. tu-a-byaà
tuà-kaà-lyaê more recent past
‘we were eating’
(36)a. tu-endïù-byaà ïù-tu-naàmuà-lyaê (more remote)
b. tu-eàndïù-byaà
ïù-tu-naàmuà-lyaê (more neutral with
respect to tense)
‘we would be eating’
(37)a. tu-enendïù-byaà tuà-kaà-lyaê (more remote)
b. tu-eneàndïù-byaà
tuà-kaà-lyaê (more neutral with respect to
tense)
‘we would be eating’
(38)a. tu-a-naà-byaà tuà-kaà-lyaêê (Recent past)
b. tu-a-na-byaà
tuà-kaà-lyaêê (Remote past)
‘we were eating indeed’
(39)a. tu-a-naà-byaà tu-aà-lyaê (more recent past)
b. tu-a-na-byaà
tu-aà-lyaê (remote)
‘whenever we ate’
(40)a.
tu-anataà-byaà tuà-ka-humïù:ra (remote)
tu-anaàtaà-byaà tuàka-humïù:ra (more
recent past)
One
crucial observation in these forms is that the suffixal H of the recent past
tense seems to affect the TAM in the auxiliary whereas the suffixal L tone of
the remote past tense affects the tone in the TAM of the auxiliary in the sense
that a H tone on a TAM such as -na- or -ta- that would have anticipated does
not. In the forms that express a remote past, the TAM surface with L as in (36a
(on á of ándïù, 37a (on áná), 38b, 39b, ). In the case of the recent past
tense, notice that the lexical H does not surface if the TM immediately
precedes the root as in (35b) unless this morpheme is -na- as in (38a, 39a).
We
would like to point out that it is mostly the auxiliary that encodes the
various shades of tense. An examination of the following forms will make this
clearer.
(41)a. tu-kaàndïù-byaà tu-a-humïù:ra ‘when we hit for’
b. tu-kaàndïù-byaà tuà-ka-humïù:ra ‘we will be hitting for’
c. tu-kaàndïù-byaà tu-aàbi-humïù:ra ‘after we have hit for’
d. tu-kaàndïù-byaà tu-anaàbi-humïù:ra ‘just after we will have hit for’
e. tu-kaàndïù-byaà tu-amaàta-humïù:ra ‘if ever we hit for’
f. tu-kaàndïù-byaà tu-ama- humïù:ra ‘if ever we hit for’
g. tu-kaàndïù-byaà ïù-tu-limu- humïù:ra ‘we’ll be on the verge of hitting for’
h. tu-kaàndïù-byaà ïù-tu-náàmu- humïù:ra ‘we’ll be hitting for at that time’
These
examples show that, when the auxiliary is kept constant, and that the
tense-apectual markers are changed in the main verb, various meanings that are
not directly related to tense are revealed. It should be noticed that, in the
main verb, the fixed H of -ka- becomes
regular in that its H now anticipates onto the preceding vowel and delinks from
its sponsor as illustrated in (b). Also,
-ama- which is normally associated with a branching H loses its H and
behaves as if it was completely toneless. In (e), the H on maà of -amaàta- originates
from the H of -ta-.
So
far we have been using the auxiliary -bya-. Notice that it can also be used in
the past tense with the suffixal tense marker -iõrá as illustrated in the
following examples.
(42).a. tu-a-báàráà tu-a-humïù:ra ‘when we hit for’ (recent past tense)
tu-a-báàráà tu-ama-humïù:ra ‘if we
decided to hit’
tu-a-báàráà tuà-ka-humïù:ra ‘while
we were hitting for’
b. tu-aà-báráà
tu-a-humïù:ra ‘when we hit for’ (remote
past tense)
tu-aà-báráà
tu-ama-humïù:ra ‘if we decided to hit for’
tu-aà-báráà
tuà-ka-humïù:ra ‘while we were hitting for’
The
auxiliary -bárá’ here is in fact /bá-iõrá/[3]
underlyingly. In the (a) forms, it is in the recent past tense whereas in the
(b) forms, it is in the remote past tense. As shown in these examples, it is the auxiliary -bárá- that carries the
tense: remote or recent past tense. The main verb carries the aspectual markers
that may modify the meaning of the complex verb in various ways, that is,
meaning that is expressed in English with an if-clause, a when-clause, or a
while-clause.
With
respect to the tones in the auxiliary -bya- and -bárá-, notice that they always
end with a high tone. Following a suggestion by Larry Hyman (personal
communication), this H most probably originates from the following word. The
following arguments can be proposed for justifying such a move.
First,
the penultimate length which is the clue for the end of the intonational phrase
in Kinande never appears after the auxiliary. It rather appears at the end of
the main verb. In other words, the auxiliary and the main verb constitute one
intonational phrase.
Secondly,
we know that within a phonological phrase domain that consists of two words,
(e.g. verb+object), a H on the initial of the second word anticipates onto the
FV of the preceding word, knocking out a lexical Low tone as in the following
examples:
(43)a. móàtwaàlaàngiõráà Kaàtsu:va we saw Katsuva (cf.
móà-tu-aà-laàng-iõr-áè)
b. móàtwaàlaàngiõrá
Maguà:lu we saw Magulu
c. móàtwaàlaàngiõrá
Kaàmba:lá we saw Kambale
d. móàtwaàlaàngiráà
Mbalá we saw Mbale
In
these forms, the verb has a suffixal Low tone on the final vowel. In (a), this
L tone is knocked down by the H tone on the following word. In (b), nothing
happens because the vowel on the following word has no tone. The noun “Kaàmbale” in (c ) is an exception to this H
tone spreading across the board presumably because ka- is actually a pre-prefix
and that the prefix is N- (/ka-N-bale/)[4].
This noun has a floating H that surfaces on the final vowel of the verb as sown
in (d).
Assuming
that it is the H on the following word that anticipates onto the final vowel of
the auxiliary, one problem remains, namely, how to explain the H tone when the
following word does not start with a H tone. Following another suggestion of
Larry Hyman, it is highly probable that, in Kinande, the subject marker and the
augment have an underlying H tone that surfaces onto the preceding vowel.
Consider the following examples in support of this proposal:
(44)a. amagándaà n’ omuãlïõù:miõ if he goes with a
(specific) farmer
b. amagenda
na mul ïõù:miõ if he goes with a (non specific) farmer
The
difference between the two sentences is that the word for farmer in (b) has no
augment. And it is precisely the verb that precedes this form that has a FV
which surfaces with a L tone.
As
for the H on the subject marker, one piece of evidence for such a proposal is
in a form like the following:
(45)
mómóà-tu-naà-lya-humir-a Maguà:luà … even
if we hit magulu, …
The
fact that the morpheme which precedes the subject marker is a repetition of the
same syllable, one might rightly assume that
“mó” is toneless underlyingly and that it gets its H from the subject
marker.
With
these assumptions, we can now propose that the H on the FV of the auxiliary
always originates from the main verb, whether this main verb starts with the
complementizer ïù-, a subject marker, or an augment as in the following
examples:
(46)
tuàlwáà ïù-tu-naà-mu-gáà:nda we were (in the process of) leaving
tuàlwáà
tuà-ka-gáà:nda we
were leaving
tuàlwáà
b’árigáà:nda we’d
better leave
tu-anataà-byaà tu-ama-gáà:nda whenever we had to leave
Apart
from -lwá-, -bya-, and -bárá, the verb stem -sal-a meaning ‘vomit’ serves also
as an auxiliary. But it does not express the idea of “vomit” when used as an auxiliary. The following forms illustrate its use in a non-past form:
(47). tuà-lyaà-salaà tu-a-humïù:ra (immediate past)
‘we finally hit for’
tu-a-saàlaà
tu-a-humïù:ra (recent past)
‘we
finally hit for’
tu-aà-salaà
tu-a-humïù:ra (remote
past)
‘we
finally hit for’
tu-endïùtaà-salaà
tu-a-humïù:ra
‘we
would be able to finally hit for’
tu-enendïùtaàsyaà-salaà
tu-a-humïù:ra
‘we
would have been able to finally hit for’
As
illustrated in these forms, -sala- serves as an auxiliary and the main verb
that follows it consists of SM-TM. Like in the previous auxiliary verbs, it is
the one that carries the different shades of the tenses like the recent past or
the remote past tenses. Like in the previous auxiliaries, its final vowel
surfaces with a H tone that we identified as deriving from the following word.
Unlike the previous auxiliaries, it is not followed by the complementizer ïù-.
It may also be conjugated in the imperative and past tenses as illustrated
below.
(48)a. tu-saàláà tu-a-humi:raè ‘let us hit for finally’
b. tu-a-saàlïõùráà
tu-a-humïù:ra ‘we finally hit for’ (Recent past)
tu-aà-saliõráà
tu-a-humïù:ra ‘we finally hit for’ (Remote past)
móà-tu-a-saàlïõùráà
tu-a-gáànda ‘we finally hit for’ (recent past)
As
for the forms in (b), the auxiliary -sal- is conjugated with the suffixal tense
marker -iõrá. Like in the previous cases, the different shades of the tenses
(recent past, remote past) are marked by the auxiliary verb and the final vowel
also ends with a H that presumably originates from a floating H on the subject
marker of the following word. Consider also the following examples.
(49)a. tuàlyaàbyaà twaàsalaà tu-a- humïù:ra
‘when we finally hit for’
tu-naàlwándisyaàtaàsyaà-byaà
tw-aà-salaà tw-a-ya-humïù:ra
‘whenever
we will have the opportunity of hitting for one more time’
tu-náàmundisyaàtaàsyayaà-byaà
tw-aà-salaà tw-a-ya-humïù:ra
‘when
we’ll have, each time, in the far future, an opportunity to hit for’
b. *twaàsalaà tuàlyaàbyaà tu-a-humïù:ra
c. twabáàráà twaàsalaà tu-a-humïù:ra ‘when we finally hit for’
*twabáàráà twaàsaàlaà tu-a-humïù:ra
As
illustrated in these forms, twaàsalaà is sandwiched between the first auxiliary
and the main verb. In case it is also considered as an auxiliary, I would like
to point out two details about its use: (a) if there are two auxiliaries in the
verb, it cannot be in the first position; (b) its tones do not change as
illustrated in (c) even if the first auxiliary is in the past tense for
example. In fact, it has an adverbial meaning, something like “finally.”
Apart
from these auxiliaries, that is -lwá, -bya, bárá, -sala, there are a few others
that are monosyllabic and that do not look like verbs. They are exemplified below:
(50)a. sïù-tu-lïù tu-a-gáànda ‘we have not left yet’
sïù-mu-lïù mu-a-gáànda ‘you have not left yet’
b. tuà-táà
tu-a-gáànda ‘(as) we have
not left’
muà-táà mu-a-gáànda ‘(as)
you have not left’
c. tu-náà
tu-a-gáànda kweàhïû ‘have we left
really?”
mu-náà
mu-a-gáànda kweàhïû ‘have you left
really?”
As
illustrated in (a,b), -li- and -tá- are used in the negative whereas -ná- is
used in the interrogative, preferably with a question word such as kwáàhïû
‘really’.
Like
all the auxiliaries we have discussed, they also carry a H tone which, as has
been argued earlier, originates from the following word.
To
summarize, the types of phonological processes that have been found in the
complex verb are the following: (the type of process is given with an
illustrative example that spans the two columns[5].)
Auxiliary |
Main
verb |
suffixal HL related to the recent
past tuà-lwáàà special status of -a-: a in recent past tu-a-byaà |
no special tone ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra |
Suffixal L related to the remote
past tu-ana-byaà special status of -a-: aà in the
remote tu-aà-byaà |
no special tone ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra |
H on the FV resulting from a
following word e.g. tu-aà-byaà tuà-lwáà |
TBU of SM, Augment, complementiz. tuà-ka-humïù:ra b’ árihumïù:ra ïù-tu-naàmu-humïù:ra |
HTA from a specific morpheme e.g. ta: tu-anaàtaà-byaà |
fixed H of ka changes to regular tuà-ka-humïù:ra |
failure of HTA related to remoteness tu-ana-byaà |
no special tone tu-a-humïù:ra fixed H of ka changes to regular tuà-ka-humïù:ra |
no special tone changes tu-kaàndilw’ |
suffixal HL in main verb ïù-tu-aà-huàmi:raè |
fixed H on morphemes such as ka- tu-kaàndïù-byaà |
fixed H of ka becomes regular tuà-ka-humïù:ra branching H on ama deletes tu-ama-humïù:ra tu-amaàta-humïù:ra |
no special tone, suffixal H of HL tu-kaàndïùbyaà tuà-lwáà |
H on the complementizer ïù- ïù-tu-abïùri-humïù:ra ïù-tunaà-huàmi:ráè |
suffixal H(L) in Aux à móà-tu-aà-lwiráà |
suffixal HL in main verb ïù-tu-aà-huàmiraè |
I
would like also to mention that other verbs in Kinande also behave as
auxiliaries and exhibit the same typological tonal processes as illustrated in
the following examples:
(51)a. tuà-soànd-iõ r’ ïù-tu-aà-huàm-a Maguà:lu
‘we
would like to hit Magulu’ (cf. -sond- ‘want’)
tu-n-aànzir’ ïù-tu- aà-huàm-a
Maguà:lu
‘we
do like to hit Magulu’ (cf. -anz- ‘like’)
b. tu-ki-s
ïõùg’ ïù-tu- aà-huàm-a Maguà:lu
‘we
are about to hit Magulu’(cf. -sïõùg- remain)
tu-sïõùg’
ïù-tu- aà-huàm-a Maguà:lu
‘we
are about to hit Magulu’(cf. -sïõùg- remain)
I
have indicated between parentheses the verb roots of the forms that behave like
auxiliaries. Notice that a suffixal HL must be posited in these forms even if
they do not express a recent past tense. In the case of the verb -anz- and
-sónd-, this is normal because they use the ending -iõrá that is associated
with the suffixal HL. In the case of -siõg-, because it is a H tone verb and
also because it is not preceded by an OM of any sort, the H tone simply does
not show up.
Notice
also that the main verb also carries the suffixal HL: the H surfaces on the
root and preroot vowel and the L surfaces on the final vowel. I have added the
name “Magulu” to show that the H on the penultimate vowel is not phrasal. These verbs which behave like
auxiliaries can also be used with the infinitive as illustrated below:
(52) tu-naà-soànd-iõ r’ ári-gáà:nd-a
‘we
do want to go’ (cf. -sond- ‘want’)
tu-n-aànzir’ ári-gáà:nda
‘we
do like to go’ (cf. -anz- ‘like’)
tu-sïõùg’
ári-gáà:nd-a
‘it
remains for us to go’ (cf. -siõg- remain)
The
verbs árïù-báàr-a ‘to have the habit of’ and ári-bágáàr-a ‘to be used to’ are
also used as auxiliaries, but they are followed by the main verb in the
infinitive forms only as illustrated below.
(53)a. tu-kaà-bár- árigáà:nda ‘we have the habit of leaving’
tuà-bágár’ árigáà:nda ‘we are used
to leaving’
b. árïù-báà:ra ‘to have the habit of’
ári-bágáà:r-a ‘to be used to’
The
verb árïù-báàra, when used as an auxiliary, necessarily makes use of the TAM
-ka- whereas ári-begáàr-a can be conjugated in a restricted number of tenses as in the following examples:
(54) tu-naà-báàgár’ árigáà:nda
‘we are used indeed to walking’
tu-naàlwándisya-bágár’
árigáà:nda
‘we would eventually get used to walking’
tu-angasyaà-bágár’
árigáànda kwáàhïû
‘can we still get used to walking, do you
think?’
3.
Concluding remarks
The typology of tones that
have been uncovered in the forms discussed in this paper has been of two types:
the types of tones that surface in the INFL of a simplex verb and those which
surface in both the auxiliary and the INFL of the main verb. It has been
pointed out that, in the simplex verb, the tones that appear in the INFL may be
of the following type:
-fixed H tone
-regular H tone that
anticipates and delinks from sponsor
-suffixal H of the TAM -ya- which is actually a verb form.
-the TAM -a- has a special L
tone in the recent past tense of a H tone verb root when there is no OM in the
form and L when there is an OM in the form
-the TAM -a- has a special H
tone in the remote past tense of a H tone verb root when there is no OM in the
form and it surfaces with a L when there is an OM in the form.
-the branching H of -ama-
deletes in an if-clause
In the stem, a suffixal HL
is assigned by the recent past tense and a suffixal L is assigned by the remote
past tense.
In the auxiliary, in
addition to the types of the tones found in the INFL of the simplex verb, the
following types of tones have also been uncovered:
-suffixal H in auxiliary
using a recent past tense form
-deletion of H on some tense
markers that would otherwise have a H that anticipates in simplex verb forms
In the main verb:
-the fixed H of -ka-
sometimes becomes regular by anticipating and delinking from its sponsor.[6]
In addition, it has been
shown that the FV of the auxiliary verb always surfaces with a H that derives
from the TBU of the following word. Both the auxiliary and the main verb are in
an intonational phrase as penultimate lengthening which is sometimes a test to
know where the intonational phrase ends only appears in the main verb and never
in the auxiliary.
References
Akinlabi, A. & N.
Mutaka. 2001. Tone in the infinitive in Kinande: an OT analysis. Ms. To appear
in Mutaka & Chumbow (eds.), Research mate in African linguistics: focus on
Cameroon. Koeln: Rudiger Koppe Verlag.
Hyman, L. 1990. Boundary
tonology and the prosodic hierarchy. In
The Phonology-syntax Connection. Sharon Inkelas and Draga Zec (eds.)
Chicago: CSLI, 109-125.
Mutaka, M.N.
1988. “The Status of Liquids in
Kinande” in Current Approaches to African
Linguistics, Foris 6, 83-89.
Mutaka, M. Ngessimo. 1994. The Lexical Tonology of Kinande. Munich:
Lincom Europa.
[1] The imperative here could be called ‘informational” in that the speaker actually informs his interlocutor that he is now leaving. (See also the use of the term ‘informational in footnote 2, about the morphemes -limu-.)
[2] Probably the TAM -ndi- derives from N-ri-, where ri- is the class 5 prefix marker for verbs and N- is the class noun prefix. This form could be derived diachronically from : tu-na-lwá e-N-ri-humira.
[3] The
verb -bya is probably /bá-a/ in the underlying representation. The mid front
vowel devocalizes before -a to give the glide in -bya. In bárá, á+iõ coalesce
into á, thus giving bá-árá à bárá.
[4] This suggestion is due to Larry Hyman (personal communication); I am grateful for bringing this fact to my attention.
[5] Notice
that the different typological processes that appear in each column can be used
as a basis for comparison with other types of tonological processes that appear
in neighboring languages.
[6] Mark Baker (personal communication) has pointed out to me that -ka- and -a- must be special markers in Kinande as they are the ones that frequently appear in both the auxiliary and the main verb, and always in initial TAM position. This highly suggests that they may be tense markers. Alternatively, the word tense marker for -ka- may be a misnomer, that it is perhaps a mood marker, or something else.